Preferred complaint strategies in Japanese and Englishby Carol RINNERT (Hiroshima City University)Yoko NOGAMI (Graduate student, Hiroshima City University) Chiaki IWAI (Hiroshima City University) |
Abstract![]() |
Situation 1: A professor made an apparent grading error
Situation 2: A roommate frequently made noise late at night
[ p. 32 ]
Method[ p. 33 ]
ResultsGroup: | JJ | JE | US |
Sample Size: | 196 | 100 | 100 |
Percentage of No initiator use: | 13.3% | 30% | 16% |
χ2 = 8.44, p = 0.015 |
[ p. 34 ]
Figure 2 compares the same parameters when a roommate is involved rather than a professor.Group: | JJ | JE | US |
Sample Size: | 196 | 100 | 100 |
% Initiator contained:1 | 44.4% | 29% | 59% |
% No initiator:2 | 52.5% | 56.0% | 32.0% |
[ p. 35 ]
Directness[ p. 36 ]
Almost 65% of the JE respondents (n=**) and over half of the JJ respondents (n=**) did not use any softeners. In contrast, more than 56% of the US group (n=**) used multiple softeners. This illustrates that native Japanese speakers and JEFLs use considerably smaller numbers of softeners than English NSs.[ p. 37 ]
As can be seen in Figure 6, the NFS judgments of appropriateness for each of the three component combinations form a clear progression from the lowest scores on the left (direct, unmitigated), second lowest next (direct, mitigated), to second highest (indirect, unmitigated) after that, and the highest (indirect, mitigated) on the right. Thus for the NFS group, the indirect complaint versions were perceived as more acceptable than the direct ones, and within each of these categories the mitigated version was judged more acceptable than the unmitigated one. As can be seen by comparing the two lines for each group, in most cases the judgments of appropriateness and effectiveness were fairly similar, especially for the NFS group.[ p. 38 ]
[ p. 39 ]
[ p. 40 ]
[ p. 41 ]
ReferencesMain Article | Appendix 1 | Appendix 2 | Appendix 3 | Appendix 4 |